#42. Nothing Worth Learning Can be Taught
Teaching can provide clues for learning but doesn’t produce learning.
Dear Friends,
I often find myself annoyed by our societal tendency to equate teaching with learning. We hear this equation not just from teachers and other school authorities but also journalists and newscasters who seem to assume, thoughtlessly, that learning is something that occurs chiefly if not entirely in school and is the product of teaching.
A year or more after the COVID school shutdown, we regularly heard statements like, “Because of COVID students lost a year of learning.” A few years before that I was invited to a PBS radio “debate” on the question, “Should summer, for children, be devoted to play or to learning?” The debate opponent was a representative of an organization that was lobbying to extend the school year through the summer because so much “learning” is lost in the summer. Somehow it wasn’t obvious to either the program host or my debate opponent that lots of real learning occurs in play and lots of fake learning occurs in school. Fake learning tends to be easily lost.
I first began thinking about the disconnect between teaching and learning decades ago when I started teaching at Boston College and began to realize that whatever students might be learning in my classes had relatively little to do with what I believed I was teaching. That concern, among other things, prompted me to invite Peter Kugel, a Boston College colleague from the Computer Science Department, to give a talk in the Psychology Department about teaching and learning from the perspective of a computer scientist. I knew he had been thinking about the concept of learning at a basic theoretical level in relation to his computer science research. He titled his talk, “Nothing Worth Learning Can be Taught,” and he subsequently published an article with that title (Kugel, 1979).
The title is a variation of the famous quote from Oscar Wilde (1908): “Education is an admirable thing, but it is well to remember from time to time that nothing that is worth knowing can be taught.” It expresses an idea presented in various ways by many who have thought deeply about teaching and learning, including Plato, Kierkegaard, Carl Rogers, and John Holt, all of whom are referenced in Kugel’s article.
A Bit of Elaboration
Like many statements that might be seen by some as profound and by others as fatuous, the degree to which Kugel’s and Wilde’s claim is true depends on definitions.
First, what is meant by worth learning? I think to Kugel and to others making similar claims, something is worth learning if it affects the learner’s actual behavior in the real world in a positive way. There is a kind of artificial way, within the schooling context, that something is “worth learning” if it helps a person pass a test and get good school grades (and thereby eventually be done with school). But that’s just an artifact of the schooling system. You can pass tests simply by parroting what the teacher said, without having learned anything that affects any other aspect of your behavior. I’ll limit “worth learning” to include only things that would be worth learning even if school tests and grades didn’t exist. The same reasoning also leads me to exclude learning that simply helps a person perform well in Trivial Pursuit.
And then there’s the concept teaching or taught. Kugel was known as a great teacher at Boston College. Students sought his courses. I don’t think he felt his teaching was worthless. I think he would say that, while teaching doesn’t produce learning, it can play a role in learning, just like any other experience a learner has in the world. Real learning (that is, learning something worth learning) is not passive absorption of information in such a way that all you can do with it is parrot it back. It is always an active process that requires thought and initiative on the part of the learner. Such learning is always a creative act of discovery. Events that the learner experiences—including sometimes words presented by a teacher—are stimuli that can help, as clues to the discovery, but those aren’t what produce the discovery. The learner produces it.
Among the things that you and I probably agree are worth learning, in our culture, are how to read and how to calculate with numbers when such calculations are useful. So, let’s examine Kugel’s premise in relation to reading and math learning.
Application to Learning to Read
Most people today seem to believe that people learn to read because they are taught to do so. Perhaps you have seen the bumper sticker, “If you can read this, thank a teacher.” Historically, this is a relatively new belief. In the 18th and early 19th century, prior to the advent of universal schooling, a high percentage of people in Western Europe and America could read, and it was well accepted that if you were growing up in a literate family, where reading was part of your environment, you would learn to read whether or not you were deliberately taught (Bowles & Gintis, 2000; Thomas, 2017).
There is also ample evidence, from research in recent times, that children pursuing Self-Directed Education commonly learn to read with no deliberate teaching, and even evidence that deliberate teaching often slows down or interrupts the process of learning to read (see Gray, 2016; Pattison, 2017).
There is also reason to believe that cases of so-called dyslexia are commonly the result of trying to teach reading to children who have not yet developed an interest in it, are not yet ready to engage their intellects with it and are made anxious by the imposed pressure to the point that they develop a mental block against reading (see here and Letter #40). Children learn to read when they become intellectually engaged with reading and are ready to make the necessary discoveries. Then they look for cues, wherever they can find them, that help them make the discoveries that ultimately allow them to become fluid readers. Some of those clues may or may not come from the words or demonstrations of a teacher.
Application to Learning Mathematics
Many years ago, when I was regularly teaching statistics to social science majors, I came to the realization that almost no students had any understanding of the mathematics they were all taught in high school. These were students at a quite selective university (Boston College), most of whom had received high grades in their high school math courses. Some remembered how to carry out the procedures, but they had no idea why the procedures worked or why or when one might want to carry them out. In a questionnaire that I had them fill out anonymously, the majority claimed to suffer from “math phobia,” which I suspect developed from the stress of having to put on a show of learning what they really hadn’t learned.
In one of my early posts on my psychology Today blog (here), I described a remarkable experiment conducted in the early 20th century in Manchester, New Hampshire, in which students in some schools were not taught any math until 6th grade. The finding was that by the beginning of 6th grade, those taught no math performed better on math story problems—problems that involved reasoned use of numbers—than those who had the usual math classes all along. The researcher, who also happened to be the superintendent of Manchester schools, concluded that the teaching of math had the effect of “chloroforming” the students’ minds for anything that involved numbers, such that they lost their capacity for common sense when numbers appeared. One of the most consistent characteristics of our educational system is that it ignores good research when the findings don’t fit the prejudices. That experiment has never been repeated and I see no evidence that it is ever discussed in schools of education.
In another post (here) I described the results of an informal study of how unschooled children learn math concepts without teaching, as a result of everyday natural experiences with numbers and calculations and, sometimes, as a result of their fascination with mathematical patterns. Without the forced teaching, they don’t develop math phobia, and they appear quite able and willing to learn whatever math they need to know when they need to know it.
In yet another post (here), I presented evidence that, during summer vacation from school, students forget some of the rote mathematical procedures they had learned the previous school year but gain more in mathematical reasoning, per month, than they do when school is in session.
Again, I think all this is evidence that real mathematical learning is not the result of teaching but the result of interest and engagement. An interested and engaged person might well use a teacher as a resource, much as he or she might use a book or anything else, but the initiative and active effort comes from the learner, not the teacher.
A real science of education would devote a lot more effort to understanding the ways that children naturally learn and how to provide environments supportive of that learning, and a lot less effort to the study of teaching practices. Education is conducted by learners, not teachers.
Further Thoughts
My thinking about the tenuous relation between teaching and learning led me early on to alter my style of teaching. Instead of providing information, to be fed back on tests, I began to present ideas to think about and to do little experiments and demonstrations in class that would promote thought and discussion. If you are interested, I described this style of teaching in Letter #31.
Now I wonder what your thoughts and experiences are on the relation between teaching and learning. What teacher had the greatest positive impact on you in the sense of changing your real-world behavior or understanding in a beneficial way? What was that teacher’s method? Your thoughts put into the comments section below will add to the value of this letter for me and other readers.
If you are enjoying these letters, please recommend them to others who might enjoy them. If you are not yet a subscriber, please subscribe. If you have a free subscription, please consider upgrading to paid—at just $50 for a year. All funds I receive through paid subscriptions are used to support nonprofit organizations I’m involved with that are aimed at bringing more play and freedom to children’s lives.
With respect and best wishes,
Peter
---
Note: This letter is a modified version of an essay I published a year ago on my Psychology Today Blog, Freedom to Learn.
References
Bowles, S. & Gintis, E. (2000). The origins of mass public education. Ch. 33 in Roy Lowe (ed.), History of education: major themes. Volume II: Education in its social context. London: RoutledgeFlamer
Gray, P. (2016). Children’s natural ways of educating themselves still work: even for the three Rs. In D.C. Geary & D.B. Berch (eds), Evolutionary perspectives in child development and education pp 67-93. Springer.
Kugel, P. (1979). Nothing worth learning can be taught. Improving College and University Teaching, 27, 5-9.
Pattison, H. (2017). Rethinking learning to read. Shrewsbury, UK: Education Heretics Press.
Thomas, A. (2017). Forward to H. Pattison, Rethinking learning to read.
Wilde, O. (1908). The critic as artist. Intentions, 3rd edition. Methuen & Co.
I had a great biology teacher in high school. He treated us like adults (so I felt). He probably taught in a conventional way. But he was wise and inspiring. Many of his thoughts stuck with me for 25+ years. For example:
- To learn something means to understand the process. It doesn't mean to memorize words. (Photosynthesis as an example, lots of complicated words which we didn't have to know)
- Only two students will get a grade that is just: the one who knows everything, and the one who knows nothing. All in between is lottery.
My feeling is that I learned the most from those people who were rich in knowledge, and therefore able to inspire.
I'm a physics professor. My love for physics survived years of bad teachers (very common in physics worldwide). What motivated me was my Saturday morning peer group where we met to solve tough problems. No grades, no extra credit to be earned.
I now see my 9yo chloroformed by math. He learned that he hates math (his teachers are into harsh, punitive grading) and as soon as something looks like math, he doesn't want to think about it. But if he approaches a problem through common sense, he can solve it. As a parent, I now dislike a lot about school.
As always, I appreciate your invitation to think deeply about teaching and learning. So as to not suggest that all teaching is ineffective (which I know is not your intent), perhaps the phrase could be modified to say “Nothing worth knowing can be TOLD”.
I teach a conceptual physics course for elementary teachers online. Because it is asynchronous, but not self-paced, I had to think long and hard about how to design the experience so that it encourages inquiry, questioning, and curiosity.
The “content” covers the science of sound, and it includes articles to read and videos to watch. But we know that simply reading and watching is insufficient - and sometimes harmful. The problem with watching a youtube video or TikTok reel is that it may leave you with the mistaken notion that you THINK you know something. What you need is the REST of the experience.
In my course, the learning experience includes conducting simple experiments (we provide a kit of “stuff”), using a science notebook to record data and reflect on what’s going on, and lots of (asynchronous threaded) discussion.
Interestingly, it forced me to change my role as teacher from “telling” and “demonstrating” to spending my time as a “Thought Provocateur”. What would have been lecture content is now assigned reading/watching, so together we can instead focus on working out the meaning together.
After 20 years, I can say that this type of online learning really works. Teachers who take the course report being enthused, surprised, and enriched - and I have to admit, I rather enjoy teaching vs. telling…!